The science of tickling: why the brain won’t let us tickle ourselves (2024)

The first thing to understand about our inability to self-tickle is that it’s just one example of a widespread phenomenon: humans respond differently to touch depending on whether the sensation was created by ourselves or something else.

If you clap your hands, then have someone else clap one of your hands with theirs, you will generally perceive the latter as more intense. This difference in how we perceive ourselves and other things in the environment isn’t limited to humans, or to touch. In 2003, a study showed that crickets perceive their own chirps as quieter than those of other crickets.

Having this ability makes sense in evolutionary terms, says Dr Konstantina Kilteni at the Karolinska Institute in Stockholm, Sweden. It’s useful to know if a sensation is worth paying attention to or not. ‘If you have a bug crawling up your arm, you want to be sure you notice that,’ she said.

Body ownership

A prerequisite to this is that our brains have a sense of body ownership, so that we know whether a touch comes from our own moving fingers, say, or some foreign object. Understanding how this works is probably a crucial part of getting to grips with tickling. Dr Kilteni says that a raft of studies began to probe this in the late 1990s, but while they established a link between the intensity of touch and where it originates, they didn’t explore the precise conditions for this. She began the Tickle Me project in 2017 to go deeper.

One of her key experiments involved looking at the way people perceived touches on their fingers using a clever set up of levers. In the first part of the experiment, people touched a lever with their left forefinger, which instantly triggered a second lever to touch their right forefinger.

Dr Kilteni then compared this with two variations. In the first, people let their left finger rest on a plate above the first lever, then the plate was removed letting the finger fall onto the lever. This triggered the second lever to touch the right finger, but crucially this was now involuntary. In a final variation, the right finger was touched by the lever without any input from the person at all. It turned out that people perceived the touches generated by these three methods as successively more intense, even though they were all made with the same force. This suggests that if the brain knows a touch is coming, it feels it as less intense. This confirms that one of the reasons we cannot tickle ourselves is because our brain has already planned it, says Dr Kilteni.

In a separate experiment that used the same lever equipment, Dr Kilteni also introduced a sneaky twist so that when the participants touched the first lever with one finger, there was a delay of a fraction of a second before the second lever touched their other finger. It turned out that this element of surprise was important; the delay made the sensation more intense. All this gives us another hint as to why self-tickling is so hard: when you tickle yourself it is hard to be caught unaware.

‘You have to get kind of rough with the rats to get them to laugh; it’s rough play they like.’

Dr Kilteni conducted a raft of experiments like this during her project, but perhaps the most telling paper she has produced came out just a few months ago and concerns an area of the brain called the somatosensory cortex, a part of the brain that receives sensory information from the body.

In one experiment she had 30 volunteers touch their index fingers together, and then separately have their fingers touched by a robot, while she scanned their brains using an fMRI machine. Some people seemed to perceive the self-touch as less intense than others, and Dr Kilteni could see that these individuals tended to have stronger connections between the somatosensory cortex and another area of the brain called the cerebellum.

Little brain

The cerebellum, or ‘little brain’, is found at the nape of the neck. It is central to the control of our bodies’ movements but it is also thought to play a crucial role overseeing cognitive processing. Think of the brain like a factory with different parts processing different information and the cerebellum is the quality control supervisor. Neuroscientists suspect that the cerebellum sends signals to dial down the perception of tickling in the somatosensory cortex when it is our own fingers, not someone else’s, at work. Dr Kilteni’s fMRI studies lend weight to that hypothesis.

Over in New Jersey, US, DrMarlies Oostland is planning to further probe this connection through her NeuroTick project. One of Dr Oostland’s project supervisors, Professor Michael Brecht, at the Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience at Humboldt University of Berlin, Germany, was the scientist who along with his colleague Dr Shimpei Ishiyama, discovered that rats are ticklish in 2016. They showed that when tickled, rats emit ultrasonic ‘laughs’ and that their somatosensory cortex lights up like a Christmas tree at the same time.

Tickling the rats didn’t come entirely naturally to Oostland when she had a go on a visit to Berlin. ‘I’m used to working with mice, so I was too gentle,’ she said. ‘You have to get kind of rough with the rats to get them to laugh; it’s rough play they like.’

Dr Oostland is beginning her project at Princeton University by making fundamental studies of how the cerebellum in mice predicts the animals’ movements. She is using probes to measure the activity of individual cells in the cerebellum of a mouse to understand what’s going on in its brain as she puffs air at their whiskers (which isn’t unpleasant but should be surprising).

Armed with this understanding, the plan is for her to then move to Prof. Brecht’s lab in Germany in two year’s time to study the connection between the cerebellum and somatosensory cortex and try to confirm whether and how the signals pass between the two.

As well as helping us build a better fundamental understanding of the most sophisticated object in the universe, the human brain, Dr Oostland says work like this could help us understand autism spectrum disorder better too. People who have an injury to the cerebellum soon after birth have a 36 times higher chance of developing autism later in life. We don’t fully understand why, but Dr Oostaland says fundamental studies like this could help.

The research in this article was funded by the EU. If you liked this article, please consider sharing it on social media.

The science of tickling: why the brain won’t let us tickle ourselves (2024)

FAQs

The science of tickling: why the brain won’t let us tickle ourselves? ›

Our studies at University College London have shown that the cerebellum can predict sensations when your own movement causes them but not when someone else does. When you try to tickle yourself, the cerebellum predicts the sensation and this prediction is used to cancel the response of other brain areas to the tickle.

Why are we not ticklish when we tickle ourselves? ›

Pressure on certain sensitive parts of the body can lead to a tickling sensation. Yet even the most ticklish person will have a hard time trying to tickle themselves. That's because our brains anticipate our touch, effectively canceling out our own tickles.

Why can't humans tickle themselves? ›

If someone else can make you laugh and twitch by poking you in the ribcage, shouldn't you be able to do the same thing to yourself? The reason you can't tickle yourself is that when you move a part of your own body, a part of your brain monitors the movement and anticipates the sensations that it will cause.

What does tickling do to the brain? ›

During a tickle, the skin's nerve endings shoot electrical signals to the somatosensory cortex, a part of the brain that processes touch. Meanwhile, the anterior cingulate cortex analyzes these signals as either harmful or playful. But in the back part of the brain, the cerebellum gives you away.

What is the science behind a tickle? ›

The somatosensory cortex is responsible for analyzing touch; for example, the pressure associated with it. The signal sent from the skin's sensory receptors also passes through the anterior cingulated cortex, which governs pleasant feelings [source: Blakemore]. Together, these two create the tickle sensation.

What is the science behind not being ticklish? ›

Some people are more sensitive to touch than others, so skin sensitivity can play a role in how ticklish a person is. A person with a loss of feeling in a particular part of the body, or with desensitized nerves, would be less likely to experience the tickling response.

How rare is it to be able to tickle yourself? ›

Most people cannot tickle themselves. However, some individuals with schizophrenia may be able to do so, possibly because they are less aware of the consequences of their movements.

What is the psychology behind tickling? ›

According to the researchers of the University of Tuebingen, the tickle, by activating the part of our brain that anticipates pain, would be the part of a defense mechanism that signals submission as a function of escape. The laughter generated by the tickle would therefore be part of a mechanism of attack or flight.

Why you shouldn't tickle someone? ›

It can cause physical harm

The tickling sensation can be too intense for their delicate skin, leading to redness, irritation, and injury. It can also cause respiratory problems, especially for children with asthma.

Where is the only place you can tickle yourself? ›

Tickle the roof of your mouth with your tongue.

Lightly rotate your tongue in a circle on the roof of your mouth to create a tickling sensation. No one is entirely sure why this method works, since the areas of our brain that process sensation are less active when self-tickling.

What is the most ticklish spot on the human body? ›

While the palm of the hand is far more sensitive to touch, most people find that the soles of their feet are the most ticklish. Other commonly ticklish areas include the belly, sides of the torso, underarms, ribs, midriff, neck, back of the knee, thighs, buttocks, nose, feet and perineum.

Can too much tickling be harmful? ›

“Excessive tickling can also lead to anxiety in children. Not only this, but tickling can even cause death from asphyxia, brain aneurysms or other stress-related injuries when done constantly. It is better to avoid tickling the baby to make sure that there are no problems due to it,” said Dr Kathwate.

What happens if you are tickled for 24 hours? ›

It could cause you both mental and physical harm. At the 24 hour Mark, you'd have lost control of your body. and you wouldn't be able to regulate your breathing, and you'd be very stressed. You could have asphyxia, a brain aneurysm or a heart attack.

Why can't we tickle ourselves? ›

Brain scientists at the University College London have pinpointed the cerebellum as the part of the brain that prevents us from self-tickling. The cerebellum is the region located at the base of the brain that monitors our movements. It can distinguish expected sensations from unexpected sensations.

What is the biological reason for tickling? ›

Tickling, he says, is partly a mechanism for social bonding between close companions and helps forge relationships between family members and friends. Laughter in response to tickling kicks in during the first few months of life.

What is the evolutionary purpose of being ticklish? ›

What makes someone ticklish? There are a couple schools of thought on what makes someone ticklish. One theory is that being ticklish evolved as a defense mechanism to protect vulnerable areas of the body and to show submission. Another theory is that tickling encourages social bonding.

Is it rare to not be ticklish? ›

Good news: It's all normal. “As with any sensory experience, people have different levels of sensitivity to touch and tickle,” says Alicia Walf, PhD, a senior lecturer in cognitive science at Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute in Troy, New York.

Why don't humans like being tickled? ›

The somatosensory cortex analyzes touch and the anterior cingulate cortex perceives pleasure. But our response to the tickling sensation is affected by our emotional state, as well as our level of attention and any distractions, meaning not everybody finds it pleasurable.

Why you shouldn't tickle people? ›

Tickling can be traumatic.

Other times, we kept saying stop repeatedly or tried to flee as the tickler continued. A prolonged tickling session can speed up the heart rate, fueling the child's fear of suffocation and causing them to feel hyper-aroused.

Top Articles
In A Standard Deed, Which Of The Following Clauses Conveys The Rights And Privileges Of Ownership? A.
Did Joe pass wind during his terrible presidential debate?
Krua Thai In Ravenna
Her Triplet Alphas Chapter 32
Wal-Mart 140 Supercenter Products
James Darren, ‘Gidget’ teen idol, singer and director, dies at 88
Savage X Fenty Wiki
Netlearning Login Rwjbh
Email Hosting » Affordable Mail Solution with Personal Domain | IONOS
Www. Kdarchitects .Net
Hessaire Mini Split Remote Control Manual
B Corp: Definition, Advantages, Disadvantages, and Examples
Tinyzonetv.to Unblocked
M3Gan Showtimes Near Regal City North
Sloansmoans Bio
Eliud Kipchoge Resting Heart Rate
Catholic Church Near Seatac Airport
Dtm Urban Dictionary
Labcorp Locations Near Me
Ups Store Pineville La
Craigslist Apartments For Rent Ozone Park
Zom100 Mangadex
What is a Nutmeg in Soccer? (Explained!) - Soccer Knowledge Hub
Wdl Nursing Abbreviation
Sunset On November 5 2023
Cric7.Net Ipl 2023
Burlington Spectrum Tucson
Academy Sports Meridian Ms
Cnb Pittsburg Ks
Sam's Club Stafford Gas Price
100X35 Puerto Rico Meaning
Late Bloomers Summary and Key Lessons | Rich Karlgaard
Shiawassee County 911 Active Events
Jcpenney Salon Salinas
O'reilly's In Mathis Texas
Lacy Aaron Schmidt Where Is He Now
My Compeat Workforce
Sherwin Williams Buttercream
Heffalumps And Woozles Racist
Linden Creek Golden Retrievers
Wash World Of Lexington Coin Laundry
Tses Orts.com
How Did Kratos Remove The Chains
Ultipro Fleet Farm
How To Use Price Chopper Points At Quiktrip
Katmovie.hs
Gaylia puss*r Davis
Rainfall Map Oklahoma
Jeff Buley Obituary
Craigslist Pgh Furniture
Bookoo Garage Sales
Job ID:24023861 - Compliance and Operational Risk Specialist - Multiple Locations
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Jerrold Considine

Last Updated:

Views: 5301

Rating: 4.8 / 5 (78 voted)

Reviews: 85% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Jerrold Considine

Birthday: 1993-11-03

Address: Suite 447 3463 Marybelle Circles, New Marlin, AL 20765

Phone: +5816749283868

Job: Sales Executive

Hobby: Air sports, Sand art, Electronics, LARPing, Baseball, Book restoration, Puzzles

Introduction: My name is Jerrold Considine, I am a combative, cheerful, encouraging, happy, enthusiastic, funny, kind person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.